PLAGIARISM FREE WRITING SERVICE
We accept
MONEY BACK GUARANTEE
100%
QUALITY

To Explain Open fire Setting Behaviour Psychology Essay

Intentional firesetting is an expensive and serious form of anti social behaviour that can have damaging consequences, both personally and financially. Encapsulating this notion, fire departments in america established that in 2007, there have been over 300, 000 intentionally set fires recorded, causing a huge selection of fatalities, thousands of injuries and generated on the billion us dollars of financial costs (Hall, 2010). The term firesetting is often used interchangeably with the legal term arson, defining the specific legal work of intentionally or recklessly establishing fire to destruction or defraud (Vaughn et al, 2010). Conversely, firesetting is the term used to describe to incidences whereby the deliberate setting up of fires might not have been prosecuted for a number of reasons (e. g. insufficient seriousness to cause damage or is not detected as a deliberate flames; personality of the firesetter is anonymous; insufficient evidence to gain a conviction; or the young age of the setter; Dickens & Sugarman, 2012). Firesetting is therefore appropriate to a wider range of men and women who deliberately engage in preparing fires, than the small classification of arson. Existing research shows that such behavior is more often perpetrated by child and juveniles than parents (Kolko, 1985). However recent findings show that approximately one million people in america, and 200, 000 in UK have arranged a fire because the years of 15 (Dickens & Sugarman, 2012), therefore recommending firesetting often continues into adulthood (Blanco et al, 2010; Vaughn et al, 2010). Research in to the potential roots of the behaviour shows that such actions are strongly correlated with a variety of specific characteristics and antecedents (Vaughn et al, 2010). What is more, firesetting is an indicator of pyromania, however as the city prevalence of the is apparently unusual (Give, Levine, Kim & Potenza, 2005; Lejoyeux, Arbarateaz, McLoughlin & Advertisings, 2002), the recommendation of co-morbidity between firesetting and more common types of mental disorder, as found by a range of scholars (Anwar, LҐngstrom, Grann & Fazel 2011; Blanco et al, 2010; Bradford & Dimock, 1986; Richie & Huff, 1999; Vaughn et al, 2010) has been preferred. Because of this, these organizations have largely highlighted in the building of typologies, one factor and multifactor ideas that attempt to explain firesetting behavior. This essay seeks to critically examine such ideas.

A fundamental basis for effective analysis and treatment of unlawful behaviour is awareness of, and comprehensive knowledge of etiological theory. This gives a core platform from which to plot the interrelationships between an offenders shown specialized medical phenomena and central mental health variables. Within literature relating to sexual offending, Ward and Hudson (1998) eluded to a significant approach to conceptualising etiological theory into two types; solo factor and multi factor ideas (Ward & Hudson, 1998). As these shape throughout the article, it is favourable to handle each style. Single factor ideas are those that focus on describing a lone factor and its causal marriage with offending. Conversely, multifactor ideas unite various sole factor theories into a thorough outline of offending, providing a merchant account of the way the factors are merged to help in offending behaviour (Gannon, Ciardha, Doley & Alleyne, 2012).

A further, and relatively underdeveloped form of theory absent from those detailed by Ward and Hudson (1998) is taxonomic classification, or typologies. Here, various offenders are subtyped into teams, based on shared motivational factors, personality characteristics, demographic aspects or a blend of each (Gannon & Pina, 2010). These classifications stand for unilateral assemblage that when regarded sufficient and reliable, play a powerful role in assessment and treatment, as well as nourishing into more comprehensive theories of offence behaviour (Gannon et al, 2012). Because of this reason, typologies will feature at the beginning of this evaluation. Before commencing however, it is effective to portray what constitutes the typical firesetter.

Firstly, studies from several scholars have advised that most apprehended firesetters are white (Bennett & Hess, 1984; Ritchie & Huff, 1999; Rix, 1994). Relating to gender, figures concur that male firesetters will be more frequent than feminine firesetters, for example one research highlighted that in excess of 80% of self reported firesetters were male, add up to a male:female gender ratio approximating 5:1 (Blanco et al, 2010; Vaughn et al, 2010). In support, similar gender ratios that approach or exceeding this are normal in various determined examples (Bourget & Bradford, 1989; Lewis & Yarnell, 1951; Stewart, 1993). Firesetting therefore, shows up largely to be a male activity. With regards to age, studies have discovered that more than half of firesetters were aged 18-35 years, compared to 31% of the non-firesetter control human population. In addition, elderly firesetters were found to be exceptional, with 4% aged 65 years and over, compared with 16% of inhabitants controls (Blanco et al; Vaughn et al, 2010). Other scholars have also reported a inclination towards children as a risk factor (Bourget & Bradford, 1989; Puri, Baxter & Cordess, 1995). Therefore, firesetters seem to be generally young and furthermore, a large volume of studies further show that the majority of which are usually young than non-firesetting criminals (Hurley & Monahan, 1969; Rice & Harris, 1991). Finally, firesetters tend to be low achievers in education and more likely to be unemployed or unskilled than other non-firesetting offenders (Bradford, 1982; Harris & Rice, 1991), disadvantaged in conditions of social class (Hurley & Monahan, 1969) and also own difficulties in forming long lasting relationships, as many are usually reported to be living alone and never to get committed (Bourget & Bradford, 1989; Puri et al, 1995; Ritchie & Huff, 1999; Dickens, Sugarman, Edgar, Hofberg, Tewari & Ahmad, 2009).

Representing the very earliest levels of theory development are typologies, which there were many (e. g. Icove & Estepp, 1987; Inciardi, 1970; Lewis & Yarnell, 1951; Rix, 1994). Possibly the first researchers to provide a classificatory system for firesetters were Lewis and Yarnell (1951) who, using 2000 reports of firesetting identified four reasons as to why fires were placed, these included: unintentionally, through delusions, through erotic pleasure and acquire revenge (Lewis & Yarnell, 1951). This early on typology was pioneering since it laid the foundations for other analysts to add to (Bradford, 1982). One scholar who obliged many years later was Inciardi (1970) who analyzed files of paroled firesetters released from state prisons more than a six year period and detected six categories of firesetter: institutionalised, insurance say, vandalism, criminal offense concealment, pleasure and revenge (Inciardi, 1970).

A key strength of Inciardi's (1970) work was the amount of participants examined, but another was that there have been similarities between two of the categorisations suggested by himself, and Lewis and Yarnell (1951). That is significant as the presence of these categories within firesetting can be associated with and backed by other empirical results. For example, in mention of their erotic pleasure (Lewis & Yarnell, 1951) and enjoyment (Inciardi, 1970) categories, these hold particular relevance to symptoms of pyromania (North american Psychiatric Relationship, 2000), an impulsive disorder characterised by powerful desire for and a aspire to associate oneself with fireplace and hearth paraphernalia, though as there are many diagnostic constraints, the prevalence of pyromania is uncommon (Doley, 2003). An additional similarity seen between the two typologies (Lewis & Yarnell, 1951; Incairdi, 1970) is the addition of revenge as a category, which is backed as a purpose for firesetting by many scholars (e. g. Koson & Dvoskin, 1982; Pettiway, 1987; Rix, 1994).

A key criticism of the typologies suggested by Inciardi (1970) and similar ones that adopted (e. g, Dennet, 1980; Icove & Estepp, 1987; Rautaheimo, 1989) was that their categories were generally too extensive (Gannon et al, 2012). Therefore, later typologies used a smaller volume of categories using data influenced strategies (e. g. Almond, Duggan, Glimmer & Canter, 2005; Canter & Fritzon, 1998; Harris & Rice, 1996; Rice & Harris, 1991), providing an empirically more robust classification of firesetters (Gannon et al, 2012). One particular example by Rice and Harris (1996) investigated 11 variables thought to be strongly associated with firesetting, in a sample of 243 psychologically disordered firesetters. They discovered four categories labelled as: psychotics (motivated by delusions and characterised by few occurrences), unassertives (determined mainly by anger or vengeance and characterised by low assertiveness), multifiresetters (largely set fires in corporations and characterised by poor developmental experience), and criminals (more likely to operate at night, characterised by poor developmental experience and, personality disorder) (Rice & Harris, 1996).

Rice and Harris's (1996) typology is possibly one of the strongest available as not only will it condition the criteria required for group account, but a lot of their aspects above are backed by empirical research (Gannon et al, 2012). For instance, the existence of personality disorder in Rice and Harris's (1996) criminal category is supported by results that show antisocial personality disorder is particularly common in firesetters (APA, 2000; Bradford, 1982; Kolko, 1985; Kolko & Kazdin, 1991). This is in addition to aid for the presence of delusions as a determination for firesetting, since a connection between firesetting and schizophrenia in addition has been exhibited (Geller, 1987; McKerracher & Dacre, 1966; Richie & Huff, 1999). You can claim however, that the existence of psychopathology within Rice & Harris's (1996) sample was expected, as it was limited by those who had been psychologically disordered (Gannon et al, 2012). Although one of the adjudged reasons recognized by Lewis and Yarnell (1951) as to why a open fire may be established was through delusions (Lewis & Yarnell, 1951), which research was predicated on a sample free from mental disorder.

Shared characteristics of firesetters discovered in typologies often help to initiate scholars to describe these through internal theory, thus our attention is now shifted towards one factor ideas of firesetting. Most likely the earliest solo factor theory was proposed by Freud (1932), later elaborated upon by other authors (Yellow metal, 1932; Macht & Mack, 1968). Here it was hypothesised that firesetting hails from either a urethral or oral fixated erotic drive. Firstly, children are thought to experience enuresis as a way of attempting to extinguish firesetting happening in dreams and secondly, firesetting sometimes appears to symbolise repressed sexual urges (Gannon & Pina, 2010). Other psychodynamic accounts made use of instinctual drives, such as hostility and stress and anxiety to explain firesetting (e. g. Kaufman, Heims & Reiser, 1961). However as there's a notorious lack of empirical evidence to offer support to psychodynamic theory (Hollin, 2013), it is unsurprising that there is little support this, or the suggestion that that those who established fires find the act sexually arousing (Rice & Harris, 1991; Quinsey, Chaplin & Upfold, 1989), or that there is a high rate of enuresis amongst firesetters.

In contrast, considerably more compliment has been compiled by researchers concentrating on the role of biology and neurological impairment in order to make clear firesetting behavior (Gannon & Pina, 2010). For example, evidence shows that firesetters have lowered concentrations of cerebrospinal fluid monomaine metabolites (Roy, Virkkunen, Guthrie & Linnoila, 1986; Virkkunen, Nuutila, Goodwin & Linnoila, 1987; Virkkunen, Dejongm Bartko & Linnoila, 1989). Furthermore, Virkkunen et al (1989) discovered that recidivist firesetters were those most likely to own such abnormalities in accordance with non-recidivist firesetting, therefore suggesting that such unnatural neurotransmitter defects could account for long term and impulsive circumstances of firesetting. However as firesetting is often co-morbid with impulse disorder (Lidberg, Belfrage, Bertilsson, Evenden & sberg, 2000), such abnormalities are unlikely to be specific to the work of firesetting itself.

Perhaps the most recognized concept associated with neuropsychological and natural ideas of firesetting, is through brain damage. Data has found 28% of arsonists referred to forensic psychiatry services got a brief history of brain injury (Puri et al, 1995) and furthermore, unnatural electroencephalography readings were found amongst arsonists also in comparison with non-offending and other offending controls (Bradford, 1982; Hill et al, 1982). Here it is believed that such injury may exert an exaggerating power that leads someone to engage in firesetting behaviour (Kolko, 2002).

One main part of praise for natural perspectives of firesetting is that they offer guarantee for rehabilitation, depending on the characteristics of the abnormality (e. g. provision of serotoninergic drugs for offenders with low degrees of cerebrospinal fluid monomaine metabolites; Jovi, Mirkov, Maji-Singh & Milovanovi, 1999). However, situations of firesetting associated only on biological causes are rare, signifying this might limit professional examination of subconscious and sociological factors that are also likely to be associated with firesetting (Gannon & Pina, 2010).

The final & most contemporary solitary factor theory of firesetting left to handle is Public Learning Theory (Bandura, 1976). The primary assertion here is that firesetting is the product of reinforcement contingencies and learning through modelling or imitation (Bandura, 1976; Kolko & Kazdin, 1986; Macht & Mack, 1968; Performer & Hensley, 2004). For instance, Vreeland and Levin (1980) propose that firesetting can be instantly reinforcing via sensory pleasure connected to open fire, in addition to the sirens, sound and crowds elicited by fireplace (Vreelin & Levin, 1980). Furthermore, as positive reinforcement does not necessarily need to be immediately experienced for communal learning to transpire, learning associated with open fire might occur vicariously through mere exposure to fires (reputable or illegitimate), or key types of firesetting behavior (e. g. parents and caregivers; Gannon et al, 2012). In support, there is key evidence exhibiting first of all, that firesetters fathers occupations often require considerable contact with hearth (e. g. firemen; Stewart, 1993). Second of all, firesetters have a tendency to be elevated in environments where hearth is more pervasive (e. g. countryside locations; Wolford, 1972), or used as punishment (Ritvo, Shanok & Lewis, 1983). And lastly, firesetters often orginate from family members with a history of firesetting (Rice & Harris, 1991).

Social Learning Theory can be applied in conjunction with earlier discussion of firesetting typologies, which a standard category proposed was firesetting that related to revenge or was anger induced (Lewis & Yarnell, 1951; Inciardi, 1970; Rice & Harris, 1996). For instance, Friendly Learning Theory forecasts that self-regulatory reactions are created as something of support contingencies. As a result, poor youth socialisation personified through exposure to negative developmental experience and role models are likely to result in aggression, poor coping skills and a lack of assertiveness (Gannon & Pina, 2010). As there is a wealth of evidence to aid the presence of such traits among firesetters (e. g. Ainsworth, 1989; Bowlby, 1969; Jackson et al, 1987; Rice & Chaplin, 1979; Rice & Harris, 2008; Main, Mackay, Henderson, Del Bove & Warling, 2008; Showers & Pickrell, 1987; Smith & Brief, 1995), they are likely to incline individuals towards reckless behaviours (Nagin & Paternoster, 1993) and start a propensity to light fires to get positive environmental control (Vreeland & Levin, 1980). Thus Public Learning Theory foretells that various developmental activities, cues, cognitive perceptions and goals shape someone's proclivity for firesetting, notably as a form of learnt hostility (Gannon & Pina, 2010). However one key limitation is that it's unclear which blend of factors culminates in facilitating other forms of firesetting (Gannon & Pina, 2010).

The final kind of theory to consider addresses key top features of previous theories to present a more all natural view to the path of firesetting, known as multifactor theories. Here, interactions are explicitly defined between individual characteristics and features of the public environmental that are crucial to the decision to offend. In this perspective there are two main approaches to consider. The first of that used Functional Analysis Theory key points (Sturmey, 2008), to explain firesetting as an connections of antecedents and behavioural consequences (Jackson et al, 1987). Discussing their clinical experience of firesetters, research literature and tenets of Friendly Learning Theory, Jackson et al (1987) designed the core associative links hypothesised to create and strengthen firesetting. Regarding antecedents, five factors were assumed: psychosocial downside, life dissatisfaction and home loathing, sociable ineffectiveness, factors determining the individuals' encounters of flame and external or internal firesetting triggers (Jackson et al, 1987). Through this paradigm, firesetters are viewed as those who, throughout their early on years experience significant communal difficulties and are unable to satisfy their emotional and communal needs through appropriate means (Jackson et al, 1987). However, engagement in flame and flame paraphernalia allows for some control to be exerted over their environment, which is normally unobtainable to them. That is in addition to increased attention from distracted or distanced caregivers in a way that flame interest is favorably reinforced. Such boosts in personal success and self esteem, alongside sensory excitement of the fireplace therefore increases fascination with fire, therefore mounting the likelihood of firesetting in the foreseeable future (Gannon et al, 2012). Jackson et al further add that negative reinforcement priniciples may play an evenly important role in developing and keeping firesetting. Here they suggest punitive outcomes of firesetting (e. g. rejection, punishment, intense and increased supervision) may strengthen the offenders' personal inadequacies already experienced, such that firesetting behaviours are persisted (Jackson et al, 1987).

A main strength of the Functional Analysis Theory (Jackson et al, 1987) is that there surely is a wealth of evidence available that favours the hypothesised association between firesetting and psychosocial drawback (e. g. Blackburn, 1993; Geller, 1987, Hollin, 1989; Inciardi, 1970). For instance, as well as being generally psychiatrically disturbed (Barnett & Spitzer, 1994), many firesetters are affected specifically from depression, low self esteem and drug abuse (Coid, Wilkins & Coid, 1999; Puri et al, 1995; Repo, 1998). Also, researchers also please note specifically that firesetters are generally socially inept and experience maladjustment across several life domains, such as education and job and peer personal relationships (Bradford, 1982; Harris & Rice, 1984; O'Sullivan & Kelleher, 1982; Vreeland & Levin, 1980). Although, several psychosocial variables are found to be common among other types of offender (Hurley & Monahan, 1969), therefore it may not be wise to generalise such studies wholly to firesetters. Nevertheless, Jackson et al's work is deserving of reward in its unification of isolated findings and hypotheses jointly into one complete (Gannon & Pina, 2010), coupled with its relevance for clinicians, assisting to underpin and guide contemporary treatment for firesetting (Swaffer, Haggert & Oxley, 2001; Taylor, Thorne & Slavin, 2004).

As in Functional Research Theory (Jackson et al, 1987), the other multifactor theory of firesetting known as the Active Behaviour Theory (Fineman, 1980; 1995), used the view that such behavior is something of historical psychosocial affects that shape a person's tendency to create fires through communal learning experiences. The theory stipulates that firesetting is the consequence of an discussion between historical factors predisposing antisocial activities (e. g. interpersonal disadvantage), prior and existing environmental reinforcers (e. g. child years experiences) and instant environmental reinforcers (e. g. external, internal & sensory encouragement) (Fineman, 1995). The latter is further put into numerous factors that Fineman (1995) argues should be explored by clinicians. Included in these are, impulsivity causes (e. g. rejection or trauma), crime world features that may provide reasoning behind such behavior (e. g. concentrate on of a particular person), cognitions and affects prior to, during and post firesetting, and last but not least any external (e. g. financial praise) or inside reinforcers (e. g. satisfaction or sensory satisfaction). Fineman (1995) ultimately hypothesises that firesetting is due to the culmination of relationships between these above mentioned factors, proceeding to recommend careful analysis of every when evaluating and treating firesetters (Fineman, 1995).

Akin to Functional Examination Theory (Jackson et al, 1987), Active Behaviour Theory (Fineman, 1995) performs an integral role in producing professional knowledge of firesetting. A particular strength of the theory was that the interest paid to a lot more proximal factors related to firesetting, as this expresses how critical psychological factors contribute to the development and maintenance of firesetting behaviour (Gannon & Pina, 2010). Away from these talents however, is the condition that empirical support for this theory has generally been limited to findings from juvenile firesetters, signifying many mechanisms associated with adult firesetting are left unexplained by this theory (Gannon et al, 2012).

This evaluation shows that most empirical work performed with firesetters has focused on the creation of typological classificatory systems and the examination of firesetter's psychopathological and sociodemographical features. These details is useful in gaining an essential understanding of a number of motives behind firesetting behaviour and has paved just how for scholars to handle the underlying so this means of such motives through etiological theory. Therefore is designed to help deliver effective treatments to firesetters. In response, several single factor ideas have been applied to explain firesetting, however they are unable to clarify the many features that typically interact to assist in and maintain firesetting (Gannon & Pina, 2010). Though, one key addition to solitary factor ideas is Sociable Learning Theory (Bandura, 1976; Vreeland & Levin, 1980), of which key aspects presented throughout both available multifactor ideas of firesetting, Functional Analysis Theory (Jackson et al, 1987) and Active Behaviour Theory (Fineman, 1980; 1995). Both multifactor theories hold several talents, particularly their give attention to developmental activities as one factor explaining curiosity about, and reinforcement of firesetting (Gannon & Pina, 2010). However, despite being the most likely method of explaining firesetting, absent using their accounts is any mention of the full range of risk factors or criminogenic needs associated with the broad range of firesetting behaviours, nor is there acknowledgement of the actual factors linked with a desistence from firesetting (Gannon et al, 2012). This undoubtedly resulted in the recent development of the Multi-Trajectory Theory of Adult Firesetting (Gannon et al, 2012) not mentioned in specific detail here, but which included the existing theory, typological, and research conclusions considered throughout this article into one sizeable etiological theory of firesetting and its maintenance and desistence.

More than 7 000 students trust us to do their work
90% of customers place more than 5 orders with us
Special price $5 /page
PLACE AN ORDER
Check the price
for your assignment
FREE